Digital Media Use and ADHD in Adolescents (2024)

Key Points

Question Is frequent use of modern digital media platforms, such as social media, associated with occurrence of ADHD symptoms during adolescence?

Findings In this longitudinal cohort survey study of adolescents aged 15 and 16 years at baseline and without symptoms of ADHD, there was a significant association between higher frequency of modern digital media use and subsequent symptoms of ADHD over a 24-month follow-up (odds ratio, 1.11 per additional digital media activity).

Meaning More frequent use of digital media may be associated with development of ADHD symptoms; further research is needed to assess whether this association is causal.

Abstract

Importance Modern digital platforms are easily accessible and intensely stimulating; it is unknown whether frequent use of digital media may be associated with symptoms of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD).

Objective To determine whether the frequency of using digital media among 15- and 16-year-olds without significant ADHD symptoms is associated with subsequent occurrence of ADHD symptoms during a 24-month follow-up.

Design, Setting, and Participants Longitudinal cohort of students in 10 Los Angeles County, California, high schools recruited through convenience sampling. Baseline and 6-, 12-, 18-, and 24-month follow-up surveys were administered from September 2014 (10th grade) to December 2016 (12th grade). Of 4100 eligible students, 3051 10th-graders (74%) were surveyed at the baseline assessment.

Exposures Self-reported use of 14 different modern digital media activities at a high-frequency rate over the preceding week was defined as many times a day (yes/no) and was summed in a cumulative index (range, 0-14).

Main Outcomes and Measures Self-rated frequency of 18 ADHD symptoms (never/rare, sometimes, often, very often) in the 6 months preceding the survey. The total numbers of 9 inattentive symptoms (range, 0-9) and 9 hyperactive-impulsive symptoms (range, 0-9) that students rated as experiencing often or very often were calculated. Students who had reported experiencing often or very often 6 or more symptoms in either category were classified as being ADHD symptom-positive.

Results Among the 2587 adolescents (63% eligible students; 54.4% girls; mean [SD] age 15.5 years [0.5 years]) who did not have significant symptoms of ADHD at baseline, the median follow-up was 22.6 months (interquartile range [IQR], 21.8-23.0, months). The mean (SD) number of baseline digital media activities used at a high-frequency rate was 3.62 (3.30); 1398 students (54.1%) indicated high frequency of checking social media (95% CI, 52.1%-56.0%), which was the most common media activity. High-frequency engagement in each additional digital media activity at baseline was associated with a significantly higher odds of having symptoms of ADHD across follow-ups (OR, 1.11; 95% CI, 1.06-1.16). This association persisted after covariate adjustment (OR, 1.10; 95% CI, 1.05-1.15). The 495 students who reported no high-frequency media use at baseline had a 4.6% mean rate of having ADHD symptoms across follow-ups vs 9.5% among the 114 who reported 7 high-frequency activities (difference; 4.9%; 95% CI, 2.5%-7.3%) and vs 10.5% among the 51 students who reported 14 high-frequency activities (difference, 5.9%; 95% CI, 2.6%-9.2%).

Conclusions and Relevance Among adolescents followed up over 2 years, there was a statistically significant but modest association between higher frequency of digital media use and subsequent symptoms of ADHD. Further research is needed to determine whether this association is causal.

Introduction

Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD)—a psychiatric condition involving persistent difficulty sustaining attention, hyperactivity, and impulsivity—was estimated to affect 7% of youth in a meta-analytic pooled estimate of studies published from 1981 to 2014,1 and the rate may be increasing in some populations.2 Because nongenetic environmental factors are associated with adolescent ADHD,3 increasing exposure to environmental risk factors may contribute to increasing ADHD rates in that population.

A meta-analysis of studies conducted from 1987 to 2011 found that use of traditional forms of digital media (eg, television viewing and videogame console playing) was modestly associated with ADHD and related outcomes.4 Digital media has since evolved. Modern digital platforms are diverse (eg, social networking, streaming movies or music, and texting), constantly available via mobile devices, and capable of providing rapid high-intensity stimulation. Whether the variety of modern digital media platforms currently available and the high frequency at which some teens use them increases risk of ADHD symptom occurrence is unknown.

This longitudinal cohort study of Los Angeles, County, California, high school students examined whether modern digital media-use frequency among 15- and 16-year-olds without significant ADHD symptoms was associated with subsequent ADHD symptom occurrence over a 24-month follow-up period. Secondarily, this study estimated the associations between high-frequency use of specific modern digital media platforms and ADHD symptom occurrence and between media use and ADHD symptom severity.

Methods

Participants and Procedures

Data were drawn from the Happiness & Health Study,5 a longitudinal cohort survey of adolescents in Los Angeles. Among 40 local public high schools approached, 10 schools were selected based on their interest in participating, demographic composition, and proximity. Participating school characteristics in reference to Los Angeles County, California, public schools appear in eTable 1 in the Supplement. Participants and their parents provided active assent and consent prior to enrolling in the study when in ninth grade. Semiannual assessments were administered in classrooms. Students not present completed abbreviated surveys omitting key measures and are not included in this study. The University of Southern California institutional review board approved the study.

Variables for this study were measured in fall of 10th grade, referred to herein as the baseline survey. The sample included students who completed key measures and were classified as not having ADHD symptoms at baseline based on the Current Symptoms Self-Report Form score,6 using definitions described below. Data analyzed include baseline (fall 2014, 10th grade) and 6-month (spring 2015, 10th grade), 12-month (fall 2015, 11th grade), 18-month (spring 2016, 11th grade), and 24-month (fall 2016, 12th grade) follow-ups.

Measures

ADHD Symptoms

At each time point, students completed the Current Symptoms Self-Report Form6 measure of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders–4th Edition (DSM-IV)7 ADHD symptom criteria, including 9 inattention symptoms (eg, difficulty organizing and completing tasks) and 9 hyperactivity-impulsivity symptoms (eg, trouble remaining still or with task persistence). Respondents rated the frequency that they experienced each symptom in the preceding 6 months: never or rare, 0; sometimes, 1; often, 2; or very often, 3. Consistent with recommendations6 and DSM-IV criteria,7 adolescents who reported experiencing 6 more symptoms in either inattention or hyperactivity-impulsivity categories were classified as having ADHD symptoms over the past 6 months; therefore, they were considered to be positive for ADHD symptoms—our study’s primary binary outcome. Studies of self-assessment of ADHD symptoms compared with diagnoses made by clinical interviews have an estimated diagnostic sensitivity of 79% and a specificity of 60%.8,9 The ADHD symptom severity score was a secondary outcome derived from the sum of the 0- to 3-symptom frequency ratings (range, 0-54), yielding adequate concordance with clinician-derived severity scores in previous work (Pearson rs ≥.72).8

Modern Digital Media Use

At baseline and at the 12- and 24-month follow-up surveys, students were asked to indicate how frequently they engaged in each of the 14 media activities in the past week (0, 1-2 times per week; 1-2 times per day; or many times per day). Each response was dichotomized into high-frequency (many times per day) vs other frequency levels (0, 1-2 times per week, 1-2 times per day) and summed in a cumulative media-use index that ranged from 0 to 14.

Covariates

To address possible confounding influences, baseline factors previously associated with digital media use or ADHD were included as covariates.10-12 Self-report sociodemographic covariates were age (years), sex (male/female), and subsidized lunch eligibility, based in part on having a family income 185% or less than the US federal poverty guideline (yes/no). Because ADHD prevalence may differ by race/ethnicity,2 self-reported forced choice of 1 of 8 investigator-defined fixed race/ethnicity categories (American Indian/Alaska Native, Asian, black/African American, Hispanic/Latino, Native Hawaiian/Pacific Islander, white, multiethnic/multiracial, or other) was a covariate. American Indian/Alaska Native, Native Hawaiian/Pacific Islander, multiethnic/multiracial, or other responses constituted an other category.

Given behavioral and emotional problems in ADHD,13 additional covariates included baseline self-reported delinquent behavior, measured with a sum of the frequency of 11 behaviors in the past 6 months (eg, stealing and skipping school; range, 1 [never] to 6 [≥10 times]),14 the Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression (CESD) scale15 sum score of ratings (0, rarely or none of the time to 3, most or all of the time) of 20 depressive symptoms, substance use (current [past month] vs past [ever use, but no use in the past month] vs never use of cigarettes, alcohol, or cannabis), and family substance-use history (ie, siblings, parents, grandparents who reportedly smoked cigarettes or had an alcohol or drug-abuse problem, yes/no).

Statistical Analysis

Descriptive Analyses

Descriptive statistics of study variables, internal consistency estimates (Cronbach αs) for continuous variables, and stability estimates of associations across time points for the digital media frequency index (Pearson rs) and ADHD symptom status (φ coefficients) variables were calculated. Bivariate correlations between digital media index scores and ADHD symptom status (Point-biserial rs) for each time point were tested.

Main Analyses

Logistic random effect–repeated measures regression modeling was used to test the association of baseline digital media use, covariates, and time with likelihood of ADHD symptoms during the 6-, 12-, 18-, and 24-month follow-ups. The primary analysis used the cumulative digital media-use index (range, 0-14) regressor and the binary ADHD symptom status outcome. Secondary analyses tested associations of (1) high-frequency engagement in each media activity with ADHD symptom status in 14 separate repeated measures logistic regression models and (2) the cumulative digital media index with ADHD symptom severity score in repeated measures linear regression adjusted for baseline ADHD symptom severity among students at all levels of baseline ADHD symptoms.

We tested unadjusted and covariate-adjusted models. The digital media–use regressor estimate indicated the mean association with ADHD symptoms across the 4 follow-ups. Digital media use × time interaction terms were added in subsequent models to test whether the rate of change of ADHD symptom status or level across time was associated with baseline media use. Additional sensitivity analyses are summarized below and detailed in the eResults section of the Supplement.

Analyses were tested in Mplus16 version 7 using 2-level random effects.17 Time was nested within students, and school-level random effects were included. Missing data were managed with full information maximum likelihood estimation. Odds ratios (ORs) or regression weights (bs) with 95% CIs were reported with statistical significance set at P < .05 (2-tailed). Benjamini-Hochberg multiple-testing corrections were applied to control the false-discovery rate at .05.18

Results

Participant accrual, sample size, and exclusions from the analytic sample are depicted in Figure 1. Among 4100 eligible ninth-grade students, 3874 (94.5%) assented, for whom 3396 (87.7%) provided consent and enrolled. Of the 3051 participants administered the baseline full-length survey, 2843 (93.2%) completed digital media and ADHD symptom measures, of whom, 2645 were below baseline ADHD symptom thresholds. After excluding 58 students (2.2%) who did not have follow-up data for 1 or more time points (eTable 2 in the Supplement depicts follow-up completion patterns), the primary analytic sample was 2587. Some groups of students excluded from the primary analysis were significantly older, had greater proportions of students eligible for subsidized lunch, and had different sex and race/ethnicity compositions (eTable 3 in the Supplement).

Descriptive Analyses

Of the 2587 students (63%) in the analytic sample, 54.4% were girls; 47.5%, Hispanic; 17.4%, Asian; 4.2%, black; 15.2%, white; and 15.7%, other. They were a mean (SD) age of 15.5 years (0.5 years) at baseline (Table 1).

The distribution of high-frequency digital media activities had a mean (SD) of 3.62 (3.30) and was positively skewed (the number of students at each use-frequency level are reported along the horizontal axis of Figure 2E), with 2090 (80.9%) students reporting high-frequency use of at least 1 activity. Among study participants, 172 (6.9%) at 6 months; 113 (4.8%) at 12 months; 129 (5.7%) at 18 months, and 135 (5.9%) at 24 months had experienced ADHD symptoms in the 6-month intervals between surveys. Media-use index and ADHD symptom status internal consistency, stability, and bivariate association estimates were statistically significant at all time points (eTable 4 in the Supplement).

Primary Analysis

In the unadjusted logistic regression model, each additional baseline high-frequency digital media activity used was associated with significantly higher odds (OR, 1.11; 95% CI, 1.06-1.16) of positive (vs negative) ADHD symptom occurrence during the 6-months intervals between administration of the follow-up surveys (Table 2). Time and digital media use × time estimates indicate the ADHD symptom prevalence across follow-ups did not significantly change overall, and the rate of change in symptoms over time was not associated with media use.

The covariate-adjusted association between the index of digital media use and ADHD symptom status remained significant (OR, 1.10; 95% CI, 1.05-1.15). In this model, boys (vs girls) and adolescents with more depressive symptoms and delinquent behaviors were at greater odds of experiencing ADHD symptoms during follow-up; other covariates were not associated with ADHD symptoms (Table 2).

The estimated mean symptom prevalence in the preceding 6 months across follow-ups by baseline of use of digital media derived from the unadjusted model are reported in eFigure 1 in the Supplement. Raw ADHD symptom prevalence across follow-ups, by baseline digital media-use index score are presented in Figure 2. The mean ADHD symptom prevalence was 4.6% for those who reported not using any digital media activity at a high frequency at baseline compared with 9.5% for those who reported 7 high-frequency activities (difference, 4.9%; 95% CI, 2.5%-7.3%) and 10.5% for those who reported 14 high-frequency activities (difference, 5.9%; 95% CI, 2.6%-9.2%).

Secondary Analyses

The prevalence of high-frequency use of each media activity are presented in Table 3 (left-hand columns). For example, 1398 adolescents (54.1%; 95% CI, 52.1%-56.0%) indicated checking social media sites at a high-frequency rate, the most common high-frequency activity reported. The next columns report the difference in mean ADHD symptom prevalence associated with high-frequency use (yes/no) for each activity. For 10 activities, high-frequency use was associated with an increased odds of ADHD symptoms across follow-ups after covariate adjustment.

Among the 2843 students above or below the baseline ADHD symptom cut point, 2777 (97.7%) provided ADHD symptom data for at least 1 follow-up, constituting the analytic sample for the secondary outcome. Repeated measures linear regression models adjusted for baseline ADHD symptom level found that each additional baseline high-frequency digital media activity used (range, 0-14) was associated with 0.11 (95% CI, 0.02-0.20) points higher on the ADHD symptom severity score (range, 0-54) across follow-ups (unadjusted b, 0.11; 95% CI, 0.02-0.20); the covariate-adjusted association was also statistically significant (b, 0.10; 95% CI, 0.01-0.19; all regression weights are reported in eTable 6 in the Supplement).

Sensitivity Analyses

Sensitivity analyses examining different approaches to handling missing data, adjusting for baseline subclinical ADHD symptoms, and bidirectional associations between media use and ADHD symptoms found significant associations between digital media use and subsequent ADHD symptoms in all cases (eFigure 2 and eResults in the Supplement). Psychometric analyses of the digital media-use measure offered preliminary support of the measure’s validity and did not find that reliability differences between digital media use and ADHD measures explain their association across time (eResults and eTable 7 in the Supplement).

Discussion

In this study, high-frequency use of multiple forms of modern digital media was associated with increased odds of ADHD symptom occurrence over a 24-month period of midadolescence. Most previously reported relations of digital media use with ADHD symptoms were cross-sectional.4 Prior longitudinal studies of this topic assessed exposure to traditional forms of media (television or videogame console playing), having collected data prior to the widespread availability of modern media platforms.4 Modern and traditional digital media are not comparable due to marked differences in operating speed, level of stimulation, and potential for high-frequency exposure. Although some emerging research indicates that ADHD levels and use of certain forms of modern media may be concurrently associated,19,20 the role of modern digital media use in ADHD risk largely remains unclear from the prior literature due to limitations in exposure assessment and the application of designs incapable of supporting temporal or causal inferences. The current study provides new longitudinal evidence on this topic using a 5-wave prospective design and comprehensive assessment across a wide continuum of digital media exposure, including numerous media platforms currently popular among youth.

In midadolescence, there is an inherent drive to develop social identity and cultivate interpersonal relationships.21 Modern media platforms provide unprecedented opportunities for social connection. Teens can converse with dozens of peers simultaneously via group text messages. Social media permits instantaneous communication with thousands. Video chatting enables immediate face-to-face interactions. Midadolescence is also a period of high neural plasticity during which brain circuitry underlying attention and behavioral control mature rapidly and may be vulnerable to exposures that disrupt neurodevelopment.22,23

The course of ADHD is heterogeneous, and different symptom courses have been associated with distinct sources of risk.24 Originally considered a childhood-onset persistent disorder, recent evidence demonstrates that adolescent- or adult-onset of ADHD symptoms that follow volatile trajectories across time are common and associated with adverse health and social outcomes.25,26 Volatile ADHD symptom patterns across follow-ups were also observed in this cohort. Whether high-frequency modern digital media use is associated with other ADHD symptom courses, including early childhood- or adult-onset presentations, warrants future inquiry.

The observed association between modern digital media use and subsequent ADHD symptoms may reflect noncausal mechanisms, including reverse causation. Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder is associated with sensation seeking,27 which could promote pursuit of digital media to satisfy a drive for stimulation among youth who have ADHD symptoms. Although an association between ADHD level and subsequent modern digital media use was not found herein (eFigure 2 and eResults in the Supplement), an association between attention problems and subsequent videogame playing has been previously reported.28

Digital media use might have resulted from or be a proxy for subthreshold or undiagnosed ADHD, which was not detected at baseline and later manifested during follow-up. A sensitivity analysis addressing this explanation found that the primary association remained after adjusting for the subclinical ADHD symptom score—a covariate presumably associated with those with ADHD whose symptoms were undetected. The problem of overestimating or underestimating psychiatric disease due to misclassifying individuals with symptom levels close to diagnostic cut points can be reduced by using continuous symptom measures; digital media use was also associated with a continuous ADHD symptom score in this study. Nevertheless, because this study did not assess diagnostic history, it remains possible that undetected baseline ADHD influenced the findings.

The association could also derive from shared risk factors. Common genetic or environmental influences may increase both ADHD symptoms and digital media access. Parents with ADHD, substance use problems, or other impulsive tendencies may apply lax parenting styles without restricting offspring digital media use.29-31 Additionally, teens who admit frequent media use may be willing to report stigmatized conditions, such as ADHD, which may increase associations between these variables. Statistical adjustment for familial factors and other stigmatized behaviors sensitive to reporting biases, including family history of substance use, depressive symptoms, and delinquency, did not meaningfully affect the association. Still, the possibility that media use and ADHD symptoms are secondary to unknown (and unmeasured) common factors that operate across multiple time points cannot be ruled out.

Although alternative explanations remain possible, modern digital media use could play a role in the development of ADHD symptoms. The primary symptoms of ADHD are inattention (eg, distractibility, trouble with organization) and hyperactivity-impulsivity (eg, difficulty waiting, interrupting others, restlessness). Modern media devices immediately notify users when new text messages, social media postings, or videogame play invitations arrive. Exposure to such notifications may draw attention away from focal tasks. Frequent distractions could disrupt normative development of sustained attention and organization skills.32 Additionally, modern media platforms provide instantaneous access to highly stimulating experiences and rapid feedback in response to user input. An array of information, music, television programs, movies, videogames, or digital social interactions are immediately accessible with modern media. Consequently, high-frequency modern digital media users may become accustomed to rapid feedback, which could disrupt development of impulse control and patience.

Even if modern digital media use is somehow implicated in risk of ADHD symptoms, any resulting public health or clinical implications are uncertain. The change in ADHD symptoms associated with each increment of media exposure in this study was modest and residual confounding could account for some of this association. Although the exposure-risk association was cumulative, appreciable differences in ADHD symptoms emerged primarily in comparisons of low and high ends of the media use continuum. Some adolescents in this study reported high-frequency use of numerous modern digital media platforms; however, they constituted a minor portion of the sample. Whether adolescent media exposure levels (and corresponding ADHD symptom risk) will change as digital technology further evolves remains to be seen.

Limitations

This study has several limitations. First, the ADHD symptom scale is capable only of identifying symptoms consistent with the disorder. The scale includes 18 items keyed exactly to the 18 DSM-IV ADHD symptoms, permitting classifications that correspond with DSM-IV diagnostic thresholds (≥6 inattention or hyperactivity-impulsivity symptoms). While moderate concordance between ADHD symptom status obtained by self-rating scales and ADHD diagnoses yielded from diagnostic interviews have been reported,8,9 self-rating scales are insufficient for rendering ADHD diagnoses. Second, the media use measure’s validity had not been previously established. Although this study found preliminary evidence of validity, evidence of the measure’s concordance with objective digital device use indicators is needed to establish its validity. Third, the study focused on a targeted age range, used convenience sampling that resulted in 10 of 40 partnering schools from one region, and excluded students without data—a group that differed from the analytic sample on some demographic characteristics, which may affect generalizability. Fourth, the possibility that reverse causality or undetected baseline ADHD symptoms influenced the association cannot be ruled out.

Conclusions

Among adolescents followed up over 2 years, there was a statistically significant but modest association between higher frequency of digital media use and subsequent symptoms of ADHD. Further research is needed to determine whether this association is causal.

Back to top

Article Information

Corresponding Author: Adam M. Leventhal, PhD, University of Southern California Keck School of Medicine, 2001 N Soto St, Third Floor 302-C, Los Angeles, CA 90032 (adam.leventhal@usc.edu).

Accepted for Publication: June 6, 2018.

Author Contributions: Ms Ra and Dr Cho had full access to all of the data in the study and take responsibility for the integrity of the data and the accuracy of the data analysis.

Concept and design: Ra, Cho, De La Cerda, Goldenson, Moroney, Lee, Leventhal.

Acquisition, analysis, or interpretation of data: Ra, Cho, Stone, De La Cerda, Goldenson, Tung, Lee, Leventhal.

Drafting of the manuscript: Ra, Cho, De La Cerda, Goldenson, Moroney, Tung, Lee, Leventhal.

Critical revision of the manuscript for important intellectual content: Cho, Stone, Goldenson, Moroney, Tung, Lee, Leventhal.

Statistical analysis: Ra, Cho, Goldenson, Lee.

Obtained funding: Leventhal.

Administrative, technical, or material support: Cho, Stone, De La Cerda, Goldenson, Leventhal.

Supervision: Leventhal.

Conflict of Interest Disclosures: All authors have completed and submitted the ICMJE Form for Disclosure of Potential Conflicts of Interest, and none were reported.

Funding/Support: This research was supported by grant R01-DA033296US from the National Institutes of Health.

Role of the Funder/Sponsor: The funding agency had no role in the design and conduct of the study; collection, management, analysis, and interpretation of the data; preparation, review, or approval of the manuscript; and decision to submit the manuscript for publication.

References

1.

Thomas R, Sanders S, Doust J, Beller E, Glasziou P. Prevalence of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: a systematic review and meta-analysis.Pediatrics. 2015;135(4):e994-e1001. doi:10.1542/peds.2014-3482PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref

2.

Nyarko KA, Grosse SD, Danielson ML, Holbrook JR, Visser SN, Shapira SK. Treated prevalence of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder increased from 2009 to 2015 among school-aged children and adolescents in the United States.J Child Adolesc Psychopharmacol. 2017;27(8):731-734. doi:10.1089/cap.2016.0196PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref

3.

Larsson J-O, Larsson H, Lichtenstein P. Genetic and environmental contributions to stability and change of ADHD symptoms between 8 and 13 years of age: a longitudinal twin study.J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2004;43(10):1267-1275. doi:10.1097/01.chi.0000135622.05219.bfPubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref

4.

Nikkelen SW, Valkenburg PM, Huizinga M, Bushman BJ. Media use and ADHD-related behaviors in children and adolescents: a meta-analysis.Dev Psychol. 2014;50(9):2228-2241. doi:10.1037/a0037318PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref

5.

Leventhal AM, Strong DR, Kirkpatrick MG, et al. Association of electronic cigarette use with initiation of combustible tobacco product smoking in early adolescence.JAMA. 2015;314(7):700-707. doi:10.1001/jama.2015.8950PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref

6.

Barkley RA. Attention-Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder: A Clinical Workbook. 2nd ed. New York, NY: Guilford Press; 1998.

7.

American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. 4th ed, Text Revision. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Publishing; 2004.

8.

Adler LA, Shaw DM, Spencer TJ, et al. Preliminary examination of the reliability and concurrent validity of the attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder self-report scale v1.1 symptom checklist to rate symptoms of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in adolescents.J Child Adolesc Psychopharmacol. 2012;22(3):238-244. doi:10.1089/cap.2011.0062PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref

9.

Sonnby K, Skordas K, Olofsdotter S, Vadlin S, Nilsson KW, Ramklint M. Validation of the World Health Organization Adult ADHD Self-Report Scale for adolescents.Nord J Psychiatry. 2015;69(3):216-223. doi:10.3109/08039488.2014.968203PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref

10.

Sibley MH, Pelham WE, Molina BS, et al. The delinquency outcomes of boys with ADHD with and without comorbidity.J Abnorm Child Psychol. 2011;39(1):21-32. doi:10.1007/s10802-010-9443-9PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref

11.

Connor DF, Edwards G, Fletcher KE, Baird J, Barkley RA, Steingard RJ. Correlates of comorbid psychopathology in children with ADHD.J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2003;42(2):193-200. doi:10.1097/00004583-200302000-00013PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref

12.

Yen J-Y, Ko C-H, Yen C-F, Wu H-Y, Yang M-J. The comorbid psychiatric symptoms of Internet addiction: attention deficit and hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), depression, social phobia, and hostility.J Adolesc Health. 2007;41(1):93-98. doi:10.1016/j.jadohealth.2007.02.002PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref

13.

Elia J, Ambrosini P, Berrettini W. ADHD characteristics, I: concurrent co-morbidity patterns in children & adolescents.Child Adolesc Psychiatry Ment Health. 2008;2(1):15. doi:10.1186/1753-2000-2-15PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref

14.

Thompson MP, Ho CH, Kingree JB. Prospective associations between delinquency and suicidal behaviors in a nationally representative sample.J Adolesc Health. 2007;40(3):232-237. doi:10.1016/j.jadohealth.2006.10.016PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref

15.

Radloff LS. The use of the Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression Scale in adolescents and young adults.J Youth Adolesc. 1991;20(2):149-166. doi:10.1007/BF01537606PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref

16.

Muthén LK, Muthén BO. Mplus, Statistical Analysis With Latent Variables Version. Los Angeles, CA: Unviersity of California; 2007;3.

17.

Vittinghoff E, Glidden DV, Shiboski SC, McCulloch CE. Regression Methods in Biostatistics: Linear, Logistic, Survival, and Repeated Measures Models. New York, NY: Springer Science & Business Media; 2011.

18.

Benjamini Y, Hochberg Y. Controlling the false discovery rate: a practical and powerful approach to multiple testing.J R Stat Soc Series B Stat Methodol. 1995;57(1):289-300.Google Scholar

19.

Zheng F, Gao P, He M, et al. Association between mobile phone use and inattention in 7102 Chinese adolescents: a population-based cross-sectional study.BMC Public Health. 2014;14(1):1022. doi:10.1186/1471-2458-14-1022PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref

20.

George MJ, Russell MA, Piontak JR, Odgers CL. Concurrent and subsequent associations between daily digital technology use and high-risk adolescents’ mental health symptoms.Child Dev. 2018;89(1):78-88. doi:10.1111/cdev.12819PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref

21.

Palmonari A, Pombeni ML, Kirchler E. Peergroups and evolution of the self-system in adolescence.Eur J Psychol Educ. 1989;4(1):3. doi:10.1007/BF03172757Google ScholarCrossref

22.

Giedd JN. Structural magnetic resonance imaging of the adolescent brain.Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2004;1021(1):77-85. doi:10.1196/annals.1308.009PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref

23.

Szymanski K, Sapanski L, Conway F. Trauma and ADHD–association or diagnostic confusion? a clinical perspective.J Infant Child Adolesc Psychother. 2011;10(1):51-59. doi:10.1080/15289168.2011.575704Google ScholarCrossref

24.

Moffitt TE, Houts R, Asherson P, et al. Is adult ADHD a childhood-onset neurodevelopmental disorder? evidence from a four-decade longitudinal cohort study.Am J Psychiatry. 2015;172(10):967-977. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.2015.14101266PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref

25.

Caye A, Rocha TB-M, Anselmi L, et al. Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder trajectories from childhood to young adulthood: evidence from a birth cohort supporting a late-onset syndrome.JAMA Psychiatry. 2016;73(7):705-712. doi:10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2016.0383PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref

26.

Agnew-Blais JC, Polanczyk GV, Danese A, Wertz J, Moffitt TE, Arseneault L. Evaluation of the persistence, remission, and emergence of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in young adulthood.JAMA Psychiatry. 2016;73(7):713-720. doi:10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2016.0465PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref

27.

White JD. Personality, temperament and ADHD: a review of the literature.Pers Individ Dif. 1999;27(4):589-598. doi:10.1016/S0191-8869(98)00273-6Google ScholarCrossref

28.

Gentile DA, Swing EL, Lim CG, Khoo A. Video game playing, attention problems, and impulsiveness: evidence of bidirectional causality.Psychol Pop Media Cult. 2012;1(1):62. doi:10.1037/a0026969Google ScholarCrossref

29.

Johnston C, Mash EJ, Miller N, Ninowski JE. Parenting in adults with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD).Clin Psychol Rev. 2012;32(4):215-228. doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2012.01.007PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref

30.

Padilla-Walker LM, Coyne SM, Fraser AM, Dyer WJ, Yorgason JB. Parents and adolescents growing up in the digital age: latent growth curve analysis of proactive media monitoring.J Adolesc. 2012;35(5):1153-1165. doi:10.1016/j.adolescence.2012.03.005PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref

31.

Smith VC, Wilson CR; Committee on Substance Use and Prevention. Families affected by parental substance use.Pediatrics. 2016;138(2):e20161575. doi:10.1542/peds.2016-1575PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref

32.

Chen Q, Yan Z. Does multitasking with mobile phones affect learning? a review.Comput Human Behav. 2016;54:34-42. doi:10.1016/j.chb.2015.07.047Google ScholarCrossref

Digital Media Use and ADHD in Adolescents (2024)
Top Articles
2006-2011 Honda Civic fuses and relays
Richmond County Parent Portal
Unity Stuck Reload Script Assemblies
Nwi Police Blotter
What Auto Parts Stores Are Open
Corpse Bride Soap2Day
Craigslist Dog Sitter
Bbc 5Live Schedule
Grand Park Baseball Tournaments
Infinite Campus Parent Portal Hall County
Shariraye Update
R/Altfeet
Programmieren (kinder)leicht gemacht – mit Scratch! - fobizz
Sarpian Cat
Nioh 2: Divine Gear [Hands-on Experience]
Jenn Pellegrino Photos
Abortion Bans Have Delayed Emergency Medical Care. In Georgia, Experts Say This Mother’s Death Was Preventable.
ABCproxy | World-Leading Provider of Residential IP Proxies
Bible Gateway passage: Revelation 3 - New Living Translation
Grimes County Busted Newspaper
Aliciabibs
Getmnapp
Relaxed Sneak Animations
R/Airforcerecruits
Craigslist Brandon Vt
Mississippi Craigslist
Newsday Brains Only
Beth Moore 2023
Craigslist Com Humboldt
Indiana Immediate Care.webpay.md
Cruise Ships Archives
Iban's staff
Missouri State Highway Patrol Will Utilize Acadis to Improve Curriculum and Testing Management
About Us | SEIL
Polk County Released Inmates
Wildfangs Springfield
2008 Chevrolet Corvette for sale - Houston, TX - craigslist
Keeper Of The Lost Cities Series - Shannon Messenger
The Syracuse Journal-Democrat from Syracuse, Nebraska
Hebrew Bible: Torah, Prophets and Writings | My Jewish Learning
Arnesons Webcam
Gon Deer Forum
Darkglass Electronics The Exponent 500 Test
Petfinder Quiz
Rise Meadville Reviews
Menu Forest Lake – The Grillium Restaurant
The Quiet Girl Showtimes Near Landmark Plaza Frontenac
Blippi Park Carlsbad
Bama Rush Is Back! Here Are the 15 Most Outrageous Sorority Houses on the Row
Dcuo Wiki
Www.card-Data.com/Comerica Prepaid Balance
Latest Posts
Article information

Author: Greg Kuvalis

Last Updated:

Views: 6317

Rating: 4.4 / 5 (55 voted)

Reviews: 94% of readers found this page helpful

Author information

Name: Greg Kuvalis

Birthday: 1996-12-20

Address: 53157 Trantow Inlet, Townemouth, FL 92564-0267

Phone: +68218650356656

Job: IT Representative

Hobby: Knitting, Amateur radio, Skiing, Running, Mountain biking, Slacklining, Electronics

Introduction: My name is Greg Kuvalis, I am a witty, spotless, beautiful, charming, delightful, thankful, beautiful person who loves writing and wants to share my knowledge and understanding with you.